Sleep Medicine Reviews 18 (2014) 435-449
DOI: 10.1016/j.smrv.2014.02.001

Piroska Sándora, Sára Szakadáta, Róbert Bódizsa,b,*

a Institute of Behavioural Sciences, Semmelweis University Budapest, Hungary
b Department of General Psychology, Pázmány Péter Catholic University, Budapest, Hungary

* Corresponding author. Semmelweis University, Institute of Behavioural Sciences, Nagyvárad tér 4, 1089 Budapest, Hungary. Tel.: þ361 210 2930×56404; fax: þ361 210 2955. E-mail addresses: bodizs.robert@med.semmelweis-univ.hu, bodrob@net.sote.hu (R. Bódizs). URL: http://www.bodizs-lab.hu

 

Abstract

The examination of children’s sleep-related mental experiences presents many significant challenges for researchers investigating the developmental trajectories of human dreaming. In contrast to the well explored developmental patterns of human sleep, data from dream research are strikingly divergent with highly ambiguous results and conclusions, even though there is plenty of indirect evidence suggesting parallel patterns of development between neural maturation and dreaming. Thus results from studies of children’s dreaming are of essential importance not only to enlighten us on the nature and role of dreaming but to also add to our knowledge of consciousness and cognitive and emotional development. This review summarizes research results related to the ontogeny of dreaming: we critically reconsider the field, systematically compare the findings based on different methodologies, and highlight the advantages and disadvantages of methods, arguing in favor of methodological pluralism. Since most contradictory results emerge in connection with descriptive as well as content related characteristics of young children’s dreams, we emphasize the importance of carefully selected dream collection methods. In contrast nightmare-related studies yield surprisingly convergent results, thus providing strong basis for inferences about the connections between dreaming and cognitive emotional functioning. Potential directions for dream research are discussed, aiming to explore the as yet unraveled correlations between the maturation of neural organization, sleep architecture and dreaming patterns.

 

Keywords: Developmental dream research, Children’s dreams, Mental development, Child, Ontogeny of dreaming, Nightmares

 

Introduction

The major components of the emerging psychological architecture of human beings (psychological functions and processes including sensation, perception, memory and executive functions, etc.) have been shown to be characterized by a specific developmental pattern. The developmental perspective implies to consider dreaming as a mental activity inherently linked to neural maturation, possibly reflecting the changing neural and cognitive processes. Therefore it is highly probable that dreaming can be characterized by a specific psychogenesis as well. The results regarding the sequence of developmental events of dreaming however are strikingly controversial and no sign of a consensus has emerged in the field so far. Even the descriptive level of dream analysis seems to be hampered by methodological difficulties, thus there is no agreement on whether dreams are significantly different amongst age groups and what the specific nature of this difference is. One way to approach the development of dreaming is to investigate sleep itself, which is the natural physiological background state of emerging dream experiences. It follows well determined and specific developmental trajectories [1-3], one could thus infer that the ontogeny of dreaming is governed by the major steps of the ontogeny of sleep. But, this is not actually the case. Although, some remarkable associations between non-invasive indices of several sleep-related physiological processes and formal as well as content-related aspects of dreaming are evident from the literature [4,5], the isomorphism between dreaming and the usual physiological measures of human sleep is a matter of intense debate [6,7]. Here we shed light on some aspects of sleep and neural physiology which we consider as facts with potential developmental relevance, without reviewing the entire field of brainemind isomorphism during sleep. We acknowledge that the cited findings are controversial, but we consider these points as potential start-ups in the investigation of the complex interdependencies between the development of sleep and dreaming. A major breakthrough in the scientific investigation of dreaming was the discovery of rapid eye movement (REM) sleep related behaviors and their associations with dream experiences [8]. REM sleep possibly emerges at a very young age since the neurons responsible for lateral eye movements myelinate at an early stage of fetal development [9]. Later the newborn spends 50% of its sleep time in REM sleep that is proven to be in close connection with the intensive neural development of this age [10]. This fact made some scientists conclude that dreaming also occurs in this early age and that it has a similarly important role in development [9]. Infants’ REM sleep however differs from that of adults both in electroencephalography (EEG) and in behavioral characteristics [11], thus its presence per se does not prove the existence of dreaming. According to some authors the elements of REM sleep gradually merge together throughout pre- and postnatal development to form the more solid and distinct characteristics of adult REM sleep [12]. This could serve as a basis for the idea that dreaming is going through a similar development implying the gradual increase in component cohesion. What we know is that the intensity and vividness of dream experiences in adults are related to the intensity of rapid eye movements during sleep [13,14], this was also shown to be reliable in five- to eightyear-old children [15]. Eye movements during REM sleep are negative measures of actual sleep need [16,17], and correspond to a reduction of dream recall in recovery sleep after sleep deprivation [18]. In accordance with the deeper sleep and increased sleep need in young ages, REM sleep eye movement activity is relatively low in children and is less organized in discrete bursts [19]. Thus, less vivid and less intense dreams, as well as lower dream recall, would be predicted based on this physiological measure. Similarly, EEG coherence during wakefulness was shown to be reduced in childhood, referring to lower neural connectivity due to immature neural organization [20-22]. As REM sleep and wakefulness share many EEG features, the inference that there is a reduced EEG coherence during REM sleep in children has strong indirect support. Since REM sleep EEG coherence was shown to correlate positively with emotions and reports of explicit face imagery in dreams in adults [23], it is reasonable to assume that explicit faces and specific emotions are relatively rare in children’s dreams. On the other hand, high REM sleep theta power, that was shown to predict successful dream recall [24], is known to be high in children showing a decrease during development [25]. Therefore we would expect a higher dream recall rate in children, which not only contradicts the previous assumptions, but numerous empirical investigations as well which suggest a lower dream recall rate in children. The above inconsistencies and scarce correlations of the psycho-physiological approach of dream research suggest that descriptive analysis of dream reports in different age groups has its own merits in increasing the scientific understanding of the ontogeny of dreaming. Naturally there are plenty of methodological difficulties in this field given that dreams are internal experiences that can only be obtained indirectly [26]. Since dream reports are told in the waking state it assumes the ability to remember and verbalize complex inner imagery after the shift between two distinct mental states. Memory and verbal/narrative skills are important mediators between the private experience of dreaming and measurable dream reports. Given that young children tend to refuse to reveal dream material when it is anxious in content [27] or is too different from their waking experiences [28] this problem of indirectness should be taken seriously. Could a child be a dreamer who is waking up in a strong emotional state without verbalizing any experience, or is it only the child who can or is willing to tell a dream? To what extent could dream reports be sleep-related experiences or confabulations of wakefulness? Of course these questions are rather dramatic, exaggerative and somewhat philosophical but help to demonstrate the nature of this problem. Moreover basic uncertainties regarding the methodology still need to be answered: What is the appropriate setting to conduct a study on the ontogeny of dreaming? Should researchers use a laboratory setting or rather a home study? When is the best time to ask the child about the dream? Who has to do the interview? Is it the researcher or is it the parent who has better access to reliable, potentially unbiased information on private experiences, such as dreams? Could only one dream per child provide appropriate data about the dreaming characteristics? (See for example: [29-31]). Given all the above mentioned questions, it is still not clear in what measure the different methods and research results reveal a consistent picture of the characteristic dream experiences of different age groups. In fact the research method chosen seems to have a significant impact on the results and conclusions, so in order to get closer to the answers we summarize scientific results related to children’s dreaming, and systematically analyze the findings based on different methodologies. Our aim is not to compare the different theoretical models of children’s dreaming (for such a review see Appendix A) but rather to give an account of the empirical studies and to critically review the available methods. This analysis is necessary for joining together different approaches of dream research and moving towards a more comprehensive and convergent picture on the development of dreams. It may also serve as an outline for future research with possibly more comparable data across different methodologies and settings.

Dreaming through childhood

Methodology

Reviewing the different data collection methods used in developmental dream studies is of essential importance if we wish to examine the results obtained in different settings with particular age groups. Also we have to be aware of the development of children’s cognitive and emotional skills needed to report a highly intimate and personal dream event, experienced in a mental state distinct from the wakeful state of the dream report. In this section we wish to briefly introduce the different methods and settings used in developmental dream research and to give a short summary about possible confounders affecting dream reports in children.

Methods for collecting dream reports

Observational studies. Observation is a frequently used approach in studies of the first half of the 20th century that focus on dreaming in early childhood [32-37]. These studies aim to infer the inner experience of dreaming from observing the children and the overlap between their daytime and nighttime behaviors. Typically the observer is one of the parents who reports details of the child’s dreams and behaviors to the researcher. Naturally observational studies have many methodological shortcomings: they are neither systematic nor controlled and sometimes rely solely on the parents’ observation reports. On the other hand they provide a very important aspect of dream research; the personal experience and role of specific dreams in one’s life, which quantitative research involving large number of subjects cannot consider (see Supplementary Table S1). Although behavioral observations obviously do not prove the actual dream experience, modern sleep research indirectly supports this method by showing that dream-enacting behaviors are prevalent in healthy subjects and are independent of other parasomnias such as nightmares and sleepwalking [38]. Thus, evidence suggests the close connection between the observed nighttime behavior and reported dream content.

Laboratory studies. The method usually consists of EEG monitoring with systematic REM (and/or NREM) awakenings and instant dream reports to the laboratory assistant personally (3e5 y-olds in Foulkes’ study [39]) or via intercom. The most extensive laboratory investigation series was carried out by David Foulkes including a longitudinal study [29,39] (children from three to 15 y) and several cross sectional ones [40e44]. Laboratory studies are considered the most neutral, unbiased and controlled way of dream collection by Foulkes [29] and many others since his works [45]. However results in dream characteristics especially in case of preschool aged children significantly differ in his laboratory studies from those carried out in other settings (see Table 1). Foulkes’ explanation is that these dream report differences are due to a recall bias towards the exciting and emotionally important memories of morning awakenings and the confabulatory tendency that tends to fill in the gaps in the storyline [42] in both school and home studies. Others point out the possible detrimental effects of the unusual laboratory environment so that children may have difficulties talking about their dreams to the unknown interviewer, the environment may disorient them and cause them to forget their dreams [30] or they may even be inhibited in experiencing the dreams themselves [46]. Moreover, reading through the example dreams collected on nocturnal awakenings, one notices that some of the young children are simply unable to completely wake up for the interview. For example it is obvious from the transcript that Johnny (three-yearand three-month-old), whose dream is the well cited “Fish in a bowl on the riverside”, was half asleep during the interview, which made the interviewer eager to handle the situation and to be more suggestive than necessary. The following is a quote from the interview [Foulkes and Shepherd [47], pp. 24-26]:

“Examiner: Johnny. Hi. What were you dreaming about?

Johnny: (mumbles)

E: What? What were you dreaming about?

J: Fish.

E: What were the fish doing?

[…]

E: What about the dream of fish? What were they doing?

J: Just floating around.

E: Just floating around in the water?

J: Huh.

[…]

E: Were these fish in a river or were they just in a bowl? Like in somebody’s living room.

J: In a bowl.

E: Where was this? Was it in somebody’s house?

J: yeah.

E: Whose house was it?

[…]

J: Just on the side.

E: Was it a piece of furniture? Like on the table?

J: On this side I think.

E: On the side of what?

J: On the side of a river.

[…]”

 table1

Table 1 Typical differences in results associated with different dream collection methods from preschool to preadolescent ages.

a 17% refers to the percentage of dreams containing family members, other known persons appeared less often and strangers were almost totally absent.
b Percentage of dreams with self movement of any sort.
c In Resnick et al.’s study, age groups correspond to four to five years and eight to ten years.

The lack of full arousal during the dream interview could explain the short and mundane characteristics of Foulkes’ dream reports, as well as the frequent appearance of fatigue and sleep as dream topics (25% of reports) of young children [39]. The phenomenon of unsuccessful arousal from sleep during the night turned out to be a reported confounder in Resnick and colleagues’ study [30], where they wanted to collect dreams in a home setting by systematic nocturnal awakenings, with little success.

Home interview. In a typical home arrangement one of the parents is trained to carry out a structured dream interview with the child upon either spontaneous or scheduled morning awakenings [30,48]. In older ages the child might carry out the interview themselves and tape record them [49]. These are the typical equivalents of written dream diaries of adults, that are sometimes used with children as well, especially under situations where equipment for recording could be difficult to access [50,51]. On the one hand this setting may offer security to the children (home environment, the presence of the parent) and facilitate the process of dream recall; on the other hand some reliability questions arise. Could a parent be a proper interviewer? Parents may feel certain expectations regarding their child’s dreaming [29] and pressure the child to serve the assumed needs. Some authors claim that parents can be reliable interviewers if they receive adequate training beforehand, furthermore recording the entire course of the interview allows the researcher to control their influence on the dream report [30]. Another concern could be the scientific comparability of dream interviews coming from different parents with various personality and relationships with their children.

School interviews. In a school environment (preschool, primary or secondary school) typically a researcher or a caregiver would carry out the interview either individually [48,52-54] or in a group setting [55]. When dealing with very young children (two-year olds) researchers might use rather dramatic means of reporting such as free play sessions [27]. Most authors choosing this method emphasize the benefits of the good relationship between the interviewer and the child, which is free of parental suggestion and expectations toward dreaming, but provides a safe environment. School interviews usually take place over one or two sessions, however some settings allow children to report their current dreams over a period of time [54]. Either way, the major drawback of this method is the time lapse between the interview and the dream experience.

Questionnaires. The palette of questionnaire based assessment is very wide. It is commonly used when the focus of examination is on a specific aspect of dreaming e most typically nightmares and bad dreams [56-60]. It gives an opportunity to request a written account of a specific dream experience, which is typically the “last remembered dream” [61-65]. The questionnaire form is also used to elicit formal characteristics of children’s dreams [66]. Questionnaires are obviously cost effective and allow us to examine large quantities of data. The major drawback here is that the obtained data is indirect and less connected in time to the dream experience making questionnaires potentially less reliable than interview methods. Another dilemma concerns the source of information, which has to be the parent in case of young subjects [59,66]. Evidence shows that parents tend to underestimate the frequency of children’s bad dreams [67], and possibly introduce other biases. Written information can be obtained reliably from children in the preadolescent and adolescent age, however under the age of ten years the “last remembered dreams” data are considered to be less reliable, mostly because children tend to use their waking imagination in creating dream reports [68]. However using an adequate sample size and age group this method is shown to be useful when comparing its results to previous findings [61,69].

Summary. After summarizing the available data collection methods, with their relative advantages and disadvantages, it is obvious that one always has to consider the purpose and focus of their study before selecting the properly suited method (see Supplementary Table S2). When the research focus is on a specific aspect of dreaming (typically nightmares or bad dreams) the most popular choices would be various types of questionnaires [55-57,60,67,70,71]. For estimating dream recall frequency, diaries or interviews on awakening over a period of time were shown to be more accurate than retrospective self estimations in questionnaires [71]. To assess the content of children’s dreams one would most probably choose one of the interview methods. With any method used one has to be careful about the source of information. It seems an unavoidable fact that whoever carries out the interview will influence the nature of the dream report one way or another. Parents may distort the narrative with directed questions; unknown interviewers might stress the child. The environment of the dream report assessment also affects the formal characteristics and content of dreams. The differences between laboratory and home/school setting initiated extensive debate amongst researchers in case of adult samples as well as regarding children’s dreams [30,42,46,72,73]. Two major studies comparing dream content in adults concluded that home dreams were usually more dramatic (containing more aggression, friendliness, misfortune and good fortune) than laboratory dreams [72,74], although their sampling conditions were different (spontaneous morning report vs. night awakenings in the laboratory setting). This difference regarding dream aggression was confirmed later by Weisz and Foulkes [73] using identical sampling conditions in both settings. Although Foulkes [42] in his systematic studies compared dreaming in children under home and laboratory conditions and found no significant difference between dreams from the two settings, results from other studies of young children’s dreams conducted in home [30] or school setting [54] are richer in motion, self representation, human characters and interactions. Foulkes considered the laboratory method the only valid approach to collect dreams, numerous other authors articulated critiques regarding its appropriateness in case of young children, emphasizing the stressing and disorienting effects of the unfamiliar environment [30,46]. Additionally the effect of nighttime awakenings on the content and quality of dream reports of young children has to be mentioned. For a list of dream studies broken down by different dream collection methods see Supplementary Table S3.

Credibility of children’s dreams

Amongst the various methodological concerns that developmental dream researchers face, the evaluation of dream report credibility is a central issue. Characterizing children’s understanding of dreaming as a phenomenon has challenged researchers since Piaget, who claimed that children only achieve a full picture of the non-physical, private, internal nature of dreams by the age of 11 y [75]. Contrary to Piaget’s findings current research from Woolley and Wellman [76] found that children as young as three years old can understand the dreams as being non-physical, unavailable to public perception and internal. These latter findings are confirmed by Meyer and Shore [77], who concluded that four to five year old children increasingly understand that dreams are personal constructions and are not part of the external word. While Piaget found that preschoolers believed that dreams come from outside the dreamer, Woolley et al.’s [78] findings reveal “an impressive understanding of the origin of dream contents by four and five year olds” [p. 27], confirmed by Kinoshita [79] who found that preschool aged children were able to distinguish dream entities from real entities. Similarly young children turned out to be surprisingly good at differentiating between reality and fantasy [80] and even four-yearolds were able to use mental categories to define dreams [81]. The problem of dream report accuracy and possible distortions during recall raise questions about certain cognitive abilities in children. Researchers approach the question of recall from the direction of memory tasks relating to daytime verbal and visual memory performance, with varying results. Although Foulkes [29] did not find any relationship between memory and dream recall frequency, Colace [48] found a correlation between long term memory and the bizarreness of dreams in case of the youngest age group (three- to five-year-olds). Verbal abilities and sociability were found to have a role in report frequency [29] and bizarreness [48,82] in the three- to five-year old groups. However, Foulkes remained skeptical about the reliability of dream reports from children under five years, because gregariousness but not the expected cognitive skills predicted the report rate, and dream report frequencies did not increase with age as expected (in fact threeyear-olds reported more dreams than five-year-olds). Consequently one could assume both memory and verbal skills as possible moderating factors of young children’s dream reports, but as these abilities develop their influence on report frequency or bizarreness diminishes. As language assists children in distinguishing objects and in structuring their perceptual field, verbal and symbolic abilities may also affect dream narrations in a different aspect. According to Bauer [83], the lack of an optimal differentiation between internal representations and objective reality in preschoolers is reflected in their dream descriptions. In his interview-based study preschoolers tended to identify the appearance (or another arbitrary characteristic) of the object as a sufficient condition for them to be regarded as fearful, for example: “His face looked ugly” [p. 72]. Older children tended to specify the aggressive actions and causes in more detail as to why they find an object frightening. This phenomenon may occur as children might not differentiate between symbols from actions or objects they represent. This may explain why most studies found children’s dreams to be particularly short and undetailed, possibly described by only one dream scenario. Especially given that dream report frequency in the youngest age group was correlated with social and verbal skills [29], it is possible that these children just pick a sensible, important or emotionally significant aspect of their dream content when they report their dream narratives (see section “Dreams of preschoolers (3e5 yolds)”, subsection “Nightmares and fears”). On the other hand emotional load in dreams may influence children’s dream narratives also in a negative way. Despert [27], in her nursery-based study, points out that sometimes dreams are heavily loaded with feelings that could not be tolerated in waking life. According to her conclusions this intolerance supposedly has a role in the phenomenon that sometimes children with such dream content will refuse to reveal the dream material. This problem has to be faced when studying children’s dreams and nightmares, especially in young ages and when primary sources of information are the mothers. So far evidence tends to certify children as somewhat limited but still competent dream reporters (for examples see Supplementary Table S1). But the question remains: to what extent does waking fantasy fill in the gaps in the storyline of dreams? This is the question that the researcher has to decide subjectively since, as Foulkes noted [29] “there is no absolute way to verify dream reports, whether those of children or adults” [p. 34]. However researchers should try to establish certain reference points which may help to operationalize this dilemma. In such an effort Colace [48,84] collected eight aspects of the dream reporting process that may be helpful in deciding whether the report is a dream or a product of waking fantasy, although the task is rather difficult since these two might mix up dynamically (for details see Appendix B).

Main results

Preverbal and early verbal dreams (0-3 y-olds)

Investigation of preverbal children’s dreams is rather limited, restricted mainly to observational studies based on Freudian theories mostly from the first half of the 20th century (see for review [85-87], see also Supplementary Table S4).

Observational studies. One of the first published observers of children’s dreams was Freud, who based his conclusions on his own and friends’ children’s spontaneous morning dream reports and words spoken during their sleep [34]. According to Freud young children’s dreams are short and simple, are based on experiences from the preceding day, and usually deal with emotions that are intensive or unprocessed reminiscences of daytime events. These dreams are usually free from distortions and bizarre elements until about the age of five. Following Freud’s oeuvre in the early 1900’s, observational studies became popular amongst psychoanalysts. Their main focus was to investigate those questions that Freud left unanswered: when do we start to dream and what nature could the early preverbal dreams have? Numerous authors moved away from Freud’s original idea of dreams having a primary purpose of wish-fulfillment towards the broader concept of re-experiencing emotionally intensive or demanding situations thus helping the dreamer to deal with emotional material. One of these early observations of putative dream experiences in infancy is from von Hug-Hellmuth [88] who recognized the splashing movements and laughter of a nearly one-year-old girl in her sleep as being identical to those of the previous day playing in the pool. Grotjahn [35] observed a two-year-four-month-old boy in his sleep and also his waking life, finding similar overlaps between his activities in the two different states of consciousness. These similarities were also confirmed by the boy’s own verbal reports of his dreams. The author concludes: “.[numerous dreams] would indicate that the child was struggling with strong and strange emotions which he could not work through during the excitement and rapidity of reality” [p. 512]. Other authors also found very close connections between young children’s dreams and their everyday life, emotional events and difficulties (see [32,36]). Some of these researchers thus assumed based on nighttime behavioral indicators that some kind of mental imagery similar to dreaming occurs in children already around one year of age [33,36,88]. One of the first observers, Piaget [37] was somehow more cautious about concluding the presence of mental imagery linked to nighttime behavior patterns. He claimed that the first dreams occur around 1.9e2 y of age, when children are able to confirm their nighttime behavior by telling about the dream in the morning. His observations were supported by a laboratory study, which showed that two-year-olds were able to report their dreams on nighttime awakenings [89]. Similarly the youngest dream reporter in Freud’s family was only 19 mo old when she spoke out in her dream: “Anna F(r)eud, st’awbewy, wild st’awbewy, om’lette, pap!” [34] [p. 110] which Freud accepted and cited as a dream report. Like other dream observations, studies on children’s nightmares and bad dreams also emphasize their importance in emotional processing and development. A good example of this is a case study by Anderson [90], who considers a nightmare of a two-yeareeightmonth-old girl as a reconditioning of a previous fearful experience. The girl had a frightening experience with a black dog one year before the dream, resulting in a fear of black dogs which later disappeared. After a nightmare (triggered by an awake encounter with a dog), her fear reappeared and extended to dogs in general. Here the dream acted as a means of releasing an emotional response that was inhibited in her wakeful life and had the effect of reconditioning the fear reaction. Likewise Fraiberg [33] considered nightmares as one of the symptoms typically appearing following traumatic events during the second year of life.

An early fusion of quantitative and observational research. Despert’s [27] systematic research from 1949 is unique in using individual play sessions as an interview frame. The study involved 190 dreams of 39 children between the ages of two to five years and found that all of the frequent dreamers were amongst the anxious children; however those anxious children, who were inhibited in their daytime behavior, play and imagination, did not report any dream at all. In the collected dreams most of the dominant characters were humans who, if other than parents, were usually put in fearful roles. In Despert’s sample “unpleasant dreams far outnumbered pleasant ones” [p. 170], and she found that two-year-olds mostly dreamt about being bitten, devoured and chased. According to the author dreams serve as an outlet for the discharge of anxiety and aggressive impulses which would not be tolerated during the conscious state. These conclusions also support the possible importance of dreaming in emotional processing. At the same time Despert’s experiences point out an important effect of emotionally loaded dreams to waking dream reports of young children (see section “Credibility of children’s dreams”). Quantitative studies on bad dreams and nightmares in infancy. Two quantitative studies with large sample sizes used parents’ ratings to assess the nature and prevalence of nightmares and bad dreams in infancy [59,91]. The fact that parents’ ratings tend to underestimate the frequency of nightmares, especially in younger ages [67,70], might have caused Simard and colleagues [59] to find a very low prevalence of bad dreams between 29 mo and six years (ranging from 1.7% to 3.6%). In contrast Foster and Anderson’s study from 1936 shows that 43% of the one- to four-year-olds had nightmares during a one week period of parental observation, where any nightmare-associated behavior was noted, such as crying, moaning or showing fear in the night. Simard and colleagues’ longitudinal questionnaire study showed stability in the number of bad dreams over time: the strongest predictor was having bad dreams in the preceding year, up to five years of age. Associations were shown between the child’s emotionally toned experiences, daytime anxiety, anger, fears and worries [91] as well as an anxious or distressed temperament [59] and their nightmares and bad dreams. These results concur with the observational studies discussed above. Foulkes’ contribution to the dreaming of children under three years. Foulkes’s [29] conclusions, although he did not study children below three years of age, are worth mentioning here because of its influence on the field. He found that cognitive visuo-spatial abilities, but neither memory nor verbal skills were in significant and consistent relationship with dream recall frequency throughout the age groups of three- to 15-y-old children. His conclusion was that the maturation of certain cognitive functions especially visuospatial abilities are necessary for dream production thus young children (under the age of three) are not likely to be capable of dreaming at all [92]. This dramatic inference caused an extensive debate amongst dream researchers over the nature of dreaming.

Dreams of preschoolers (3-5 y-olds)

Preschoolers’ dreams, due to verbal improvements, are well studied using various methods including laboratory interviews [29,39-43,92], home dream interviews [30,48], questionnaires [66,93] and kindergarten interviews [27,48,52,54,63,83].

Dream recall and report length. Foulkes in his laboratory studies found the dream reports of three- to five-year-olds to be infrequent (17% of REM awakenings) and brief (average 14 words), usually without a narrative or storyline. Two studies conducted in home setting yielded somewhat different results. In Colace’s [48] study the dreams tended to be longer (mean word count: 35 words). Resnick and colleagues [30] found no difference in dream recall frequency between the fourto five- and eight- to ten-year-old age groups (56% and 57%, respectively). Even though a direct comparison is problematic, because of the differences in dream collection methods, it may be worth looking at the striking difference between 17% of laboratory recall and 56% of home recall within the same age group. Although school interview setting usually does not provide a dream recall frequency measure, studies found children’s dream reports to be “simple” and “short” [27], with an average of 33 words in Honig’s research [54], that tend to be rather similar to those of the home based studies. The only questionnaire based dream study that was not specifically focused on nightmares and bad dreams of children was carried out by Colace [66]. In his parent-recorded questionnaire he assessed dream attitudes, dream frequencies and characteristics of the last reported dream of children between three and nine years. He found that 60% of the three- to five-year-olds reported at least one dream to their parents in the last month. Most of the parents rated their children’s dream reports as being short stories (57.6%, rather than short or long sentences: 32.6%).

General content. Laboratory studies showed that the dreams of young children are rather mundane and simple in their content. Dream reports usually lacked movements, actions (static imagery), an active self character, human characters, interactions and feelings. Instead children frequently dreamt about body-state themes, especially those relating to a sleeping self, and about animals. Typical dreams of this age were “I was sleeping in the bathtub” or “I was sleeping in the co-co stand, where you get Coke from”. According to Foulkes the strikingly barren nature of these dream reports represents children’s habitual dream life rather than spontaneous morning dream reports that are selected by recall bias towards the exciting and emotionally important memories showing dreams much more colorful than they usually are [42]. Regarding home interviews, one of the most striking differences from laboratory studies is the frequency of active self-participation in the dreams, which reached 85% in the younger age group [30]. These results were confirmed by Colace, who found that 68% of the dreams contained an active self in an overall sample of home and school interviews of three- to seven-year-olds [94]. Resnick also found that the most frequent characters in young children’s dreams were family members (29% of all characters) and other known children (28%), contrary to Foulkes who found that human representation was rather scarce in this age group. To illustrate the above, we cite the dream report of a three-year- and six-month-old child: “I dreamed that I woke you up [the mother] and caressed you, gave you a little kiss and hugged you, and then gave a kiss to dad.” [Colace [48], p. 105]. Studies using school interviews agree that most dreams of twoto five-year-olds contain an active self (59.4% [54], 72% among three- to six-year-olds [95]) and human characters (80% [53], main characters were family members in 30%, strangers in 10.5% and friends in 3.5%, while 43% included animal characters [54]), that almost all of them depict motion and activities (81.2% [54]), and that feelings appearing in the dreams are common (in 75.9% [54]). An early school interview study from 1933 examining the wishes, fears and dreams of 400 children concluded that children’s dreams are strongly correlated to their wakeful life and rather reflect the children’s fears than being wish fulfillments [96]. These results confirm home studies rather than laboratory results. Below is the dream report of a three-yearefive-month-old boy who dreams of seeing his deceased grandmother in the form of a soft toy, demonstrating the emotional relevance and the bizarreness that young children’s dreams can include: “I dreamed the bunny and the she-bunny, now the she-bunny was grannie and she was with C [the boy’s younger sister] and the blue bunny was with me.” [Colace [48], p. 171]. Colace’s parental questionnaire study [66,97] showed that dream characters are most frequently family members (present in 60% of the dreams), active self-representation is predominant (56%), social interaction is frequent (in 67.4% of all reported dreams) even in young children’s dreams. Aggressive content was rare (in 17.9% of the dreams), which might be explained by the parent’s possible bias towards presenting more pleasant dreams.

Dream bizarreness. In Foulkes’ laboratory studies the question of dream bizarreness was addressed by the measures of character and setting distortion, by which means he found no bizarre elements in young children’s dreams. Another study (using narrative analyses), based on the school interview of 369 children, found that in the course of three to five years of age dream narratives have an increase in conflict content and a decrease in non-conflict and realistic content [98]. Though Colace and colleagues [48] found most of young children’s dream reports being ordinary and realistic (68% on average across his school and home based studies), still 32% of these dreams showed some level of strange and/or bizarre content. In his studies, Colace used his own measure of bizarreness based on Freudian principles [99]. Similarly, in her home based dream research Resnick also found that 34% of the reports contained bizarre elements among the four- to five-year-olds using Hobson’s rating system [100]. As for the questionnaire studies, Colace found that the majority of the dreams (55.4%) contained some strange elements (38.2%) or bizarre and improbable content (16.2%). The remaining 45.6% of the dreams was “ordinary and realistic”. It is interesting to note that amongst the three categories of bizarreness (discontinuities, incongruities, and uncertainties) used in Resnick’s study, uncertainties were totally absent among preschoolers, while in the older age group it counted for one third of the bizarreness scores. Possible hints for the relative absence of bizarre elements [29,30,48], and especially the lack of uncertainties [30] in young children’s dreams lie in Bauer’s observation of undeveloped symbolization (see section “Credibility of children’s dreams”) and also as DeMartino [28] points out, in the possible failure to report material in their dreams that are contrary to their own experiences.

Nightmares and fears. Muris and his colleagues [52] interviewed children in their schools and found that typical scary dreams of preschoolers are about imaginary creatures, personal harm and animals. In Hawkins and Williams’ study [93] frequent nightmares turned out to be associated with fears of going to bed, night terrors, snoring and sleep talking, but showed little or no relationship with life events and behavioral problems. Bauer [83] aimed to depict developmental patterns in children’s fears, including frightening dreams, by interviewing children between four and 12 y of age. 74% of the preschoolers (4-6 y-olds) reported having scary dreams. Preschoolers tended to have fears of imaginary creatures and poorly defined phenomena, whereas 10-12 y-old school children reported much more specific and realistic fears, involving body injuries and physical danger. The same pattern of children’s fears was described in Jersild’s [96] study, who also found (similarly to Hawkins and Williams [93]) that children’s unpleasant dreams reflect these subjective fears, rather than their objective life experiences. Since the nature of fears and bad dreams seem to be closely connected, the above mentioned pattern of fears might be linked to children’s dream reports through linguistic and symbolic development, as described in the “Credibility of children’s dreams” section.

Children’s dreams in primary school age (5-9 y-olds)

In this age range, as children develop in their cognitive skills, their dream reports become more and more reliable [29], making it possible to assess dreams written directly by the children [51,63,64].

Dream recall frequency and report length. According to Foulkes [29], the strongest dream quality changes occur around the ages of seven and eight, when the children’s dream reports get more frequent (43%) and become significantly longer (median: 41 words), showing more complex narrative structure. In this age group dream recall frequency was correlated reliably with visuo-spatial skills, which lead Foulkes to conclude that the development of this domain makes dreaming possible. Surprisingly Foulkes contradicted his previous findings with adolescents [41], and claimed that dream recall frequency was not associated with the adjustment of waking anxiety, thus concluded that it is not personal problems or conflicts that prompt children’s dreaming, rather it is the cognitive competencies that allow them to be more accomplished dreamers. Some of Foulkes’ results are confirmed by home and school based studies as well. For example Colace also found an increase in the narrative complexity and dream report length compared to three- to-five-year-olds (median: 41-46 words), although this is a modest increase compared to the laboratory results. On the contrary, during his questionnaire study he did not find increase in dream report length, presumably because the five-level scale filled out by the parents did not turn out to be sensitive enough for detecting fine differences (parents most commonly chose “short stories” to describe their children’s dream report lengths). Oberst [64] collected dream accounts using the “last remembered dream” method from 120 children aged seven to 18 y. She found that the mean dream report length amongst the seven- to eight-year-olds was 70 words, which is higher than that of the above mentioned studies, but also her age range is narrower.

General content. In Foulkes’ laboratory studies the greatest improvement around the age of eight is the first appearance of active self-representation together with thoughts (10% of all reports) and feelings in dreams. He also observed kinematic imagery and social interactions for the first time in dreams collected between five to seven years. In contrast, Resnick found no significant difference in active self representation between her four- to five(in 85% of the dreams) and eight- to ten-year-old (89%) age groups. Oberst’s [64] results with the “last remembered dream” method indicate that gender differences characteristic to the adult population [69] start to emerge even in her youngest age group (7e8 y) and develop throughout adolescence to adulthood. She found that boys tend to dream more about male characters, whereas girls have a more balanced character ratio (male/female character ratio: 79% and 38%, respectively), and for most of the groups boys show more physical aggression and aggressive interactions (aggression/character index: 61% for boys and 24% for girls). All aggression variables were highest in the youngest age group (60e87% of dreams contained at least one aggressive interaction), who were also more often victims than aggressors in their dreams (victimization in 87e 90% of all dreams with aggression); both tendencies decreased with age.

Bizarreness in dreams. Distortion in characters and settings or bizarreness was still quite rare between five and nine years in Foulkes’ studies. In contrast Colace [48] found that almost half of the children (47%) between five and seven reported relatively complex dream narratives and 58% of the reports contained at least one bizarre element. Bizarreness in the dream reports correlated with various cognitive abilities (linguistic skills, attention span, symbolization and visuo-spatial skills) and superego development (according to Freudian structural theory [34]). Superego development was measured by performance in situational stories about social normativity [48]. Colace’s school based studies yielded similar results. He concluded that the developmental achievement that allows dreams to show a highly bizarre narrative can be present at the age of five: “There was a horse, it was all green with red eyes, so this horse took mum, [.] she was the horse’s wife [.] my eyes became red because I was the daughter of these two people, . of this horse and of this and of . mother” [dream report of a five-yeareten-month-old girl, Colace [48], p. 120].

Nightmares and trauma. Assessing the relationships between waking life and nightmares Li and colleagues [60] found that frequent nightmares were associated with a constellation of child, sleep and family related factors, such as comorbid sleep disturbances, parental predisposition, child hyperactivity, mood disturbances and poor academic performance. Extreme conditions in waking life, such as war trauma, have a distinctive effect on children’s dreams, as high levels of aggression and death scenes appeared in dreams of children reported in an early study conducted during the First Word War [63]. Helminen and Punamäki [51] also found a strong effect by examining the dreams of Palestinian children from traumatic and non-traumatic environments between the ages of six to 16 y. They found enhanced dream recall (mean dream recall in the trauma group: 4.13 versus non-trauma group: 2.89) with more contextual images (90% versus 74%) among the highly traumatized children. Exposure to severe trauma was usually associated with a higher level of posttraumatic symptoms and a high intensity of negative emotional imagery in dreams, however this was not the case for the children in the trauma group whose dreams incorporated highly intense positive emotional imagery. This relationship between the emotional processing of traumatic events and dreams is also shown in the dreams of children who have suffered road traffic accidents [101]: between two and six months post-accident, the incidence of posttraumatic nightmares decreased in parallel to the decline in posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) symptoms. Similarly Terr [102], dealing with children who had been kidnapped, found that all of the children had had dreams about the traumatic event, including unremembered night terrors (60% of the children), exact repeat dreams (52%), modified playback dreams (52%) and disguised dreams (17%) that incorporated symbolically some aspects of the event. He found that those children who had good abilities to verbalize their feelings tended to have more variety of dreams related to the trauma, had more memories of their dreams and could use their associations to gain relief in their psychiatric interviews. Studies assessing nightmares and bad dreams of school aged children emphasize the association between feelings, emotional regulation, coping mechanisms and dreams, and conclude that dreams are correlated with [60,91] or even promote emotional processing and work-through [51,83,102].

Dreaming in preadolescence (9-14 y-olds)

In the pre-adolescent age questionnaires and written dream accounts are the most popular dream collection methods, since they provide the easiest way to collect a large amount of data from numerous participants (see section “Methods for collecting dream reports”).

Dream recall frequency and report length. Foulkes found that between the ages of nine to 11 dream report frequency of REM awakenings reached a median of 79%, close to the typical adult data (85e90%), with both report frequency and length becoming stable individual parameters for each child. Together with these changes, visuo-spatial skills, although still associated with report frequency, had relatively less influence on dreaming than other personal/social variables, similarly to adults [29]. According to another laboratory based study by Strauch [103] children still continue to report more dreams upon night awakenings between the ages of 9e15 (increase from 77% to 82% for girls and from 58% to 74% to boys). Strauch, in her home based study [49], where children had to tape record their own verbal dream recalls, found report length improve from an average of 100 words to 141 words from nine to 15 y of age. Soffer-Dudek [71] in her seven-day dream frequency log-based longitudinal study of children aged ten to twelve found an average of one dream recalled in every second night. She also observed a distinct decreasing tendency in girls dream report frequency over the years.

General content of dreams. According to Foulkes’ findings in this age range dreaming starts to reflect personality; for instance children who frequently dream of an angry self, display hostility during the presleep period as well. Along with personality, gender roles seem to be reflected in dreams starting at preadolescence [104]. An interesting pattern that arises among the 13e15 y-olds is that a number of the REM dream reports seem to lose some of the achieved vividness, kinematic characteristics, social interaction, busyness and narrative complexity, and become more similar to the simpler non-REM dream reports. This phenomenon, together with the lower report rate (73%) compared to the 9e11 y-olds, could be connected to ongoing neural changes (synaptic pruning) in the adolescent age, as Soffer-Dudek also hypothesizes based on similar recall frequency patterns in her questionnaire based work [71]. Similarly in her laboratory study, Strauch [103] found an almost continuous increase in dream report frequency from 9e15 y, with a slight relapse from 79% in the 9e11 to 74% in the 11e13 y age group for girls. Other aspects of her findings (both in the laboratory and in her home study [49]) are in line with Foulkes’ results: gradual appearance of active self, increase in social interactions, inclusion of speech and relative scarceness of feelings associated with dreams. Overall aggression/friendliness percentage declined for boys (70% at 9e11 y) and increased for girls (36% at 9e11 y), arriving at around the same level at 11e15 y (51% and 61% respectively). All children tended to be victims of aggression rather than being aggressors. Similarly, in 10e12 y-old girls’ dreams (collected from www. DreamBank.net [68]) McNamara and colleagues found that a high percentage depicted the dreamer as a victim. Moreover, a high self negativity together with low self agency (the latter measured by dreamer involved success percent: 35% compared to 63% of adults) was also reported for this age group [105]. Findings using the “last remembered dream” method show many similarities with laboratory studies [61,62,65], most interestingly about aggression and gender differences that become more prominent in the preadolescent age [62,65]. Dream contents like aggressiveness, inhibition of aggression, friendliness and dream emotions were shown to correlate with personality traits such as neuroticism and extraversion in a dream content questionnaire-based study of 107 preadolescent students [106].

Bizarreness. Dream bizarreness seemed to change continuously throughout the age range [103]: bizarre or unlikely dreams (lacking any relation to the waking world) decreased with age (from 31% to 15% of all dreams), but inventive dreams (combining waking experiences in an unusual manner) increased (from 29% to 44%) showing the development of higher level cognitive skills, as separate waking memories had to be combined into new entities.

Nightmare studies. Dream diaries are typically used to assess the dreams of children living under traumatizing conditions. Punamäki and colleagues [50,51,107] found that children who had waking traumatic experiences reported more unpleasant, mundane, and fragmented dreams involving death and destruction themes, with typical feelings of anger, anxiety and hostility. Bilu [108], examining Israeli and Arab children’s dreams found similar results: children living closer to disturbed areas or the “enemy” had significantly more “encounter dreams” involving meeting characters from the other side pervaded with aggression and overt violence. Valli et al. compared the dream logs of Kurdish children and adolescents (9-17 y-olds) severely traumatized by Iraqi military forces with similar data derived from non-traumatized Finnish children. Significantly higher number of dreams and threatening dream events were evident in the dream reports of the traumatized children. Moreover, the dream threats of traumatized children were also more severe in nature than the threats of less traumatized or nontraumatized children [109]. Levine [110], however, studying Irish, Bedouin and Israeli children’s dreams, found that culture and norms had a stronger effect on dreams than the closeness or exposure to conflicts.

Questionnaire based nightmare and bad dream studies typically show that nightmare frequency is highest between the ages of five to ten [60,111,112], and is related to other sleep disorders [56,60], trait anxiety [56,57,113], emotional problems [67,70,71], accumulated stress in wakeful life [56] and behavior problems [60,114]. The strongest predictor was having nightmares at a previous testing time [59,70]. Similarly to questionnaire based findings [59,70], Foulkes also found that the frequency of unpleasant dreams became stable within this age span, so that the number of such dreams between nine to 11 y also predicted the same prevalence five years later. Although nightmares are shown to be a stable feature of childhood, investigations may show some influence of television watching on aggressive and scary dreams [115]. It is still not clear if nightmare frequency is affected by television or whether bad dreams take up the program content, but nightmare content seems to change with the popular scary figure of the time [116].

Adolescent dreams (14-18 y)

Since we aimed to focus on the developmental aspects of dreaming we do not discuss adolescents’ dreams here because they are rather similar to those of the adults’, considering both research methodologies and results. As an end of the gradual increase during childhood, dream report characteristics in adolescence including gender differences tend to arrive to the same level as that of the adult population [64], presumably reflecting stronger socialization effects [117].

 

Summary and conclusions

Looking at the available data on children’s dreams it is obvious that different dream collection methods often result in highly variable and sometimes even contradictory outcomes. Studying young children’s dreams the number of available methods is restricted, this age group shows the most diverse results, probably due to higher sensitivity to different environmental conditions and research settings. In this segment we aim to highlight the most important results on and discuss the relevant aspects of children’s dreaming, to draw implications for present dream theory and guidelines for future research.

Children’s dream reports

Given the numerous controversies in the field it is surprising that children’s dream narratives being short and simple (compared to those of the adults) is a feature that has been confirmed by several authors [27,29,37,48,66] after the first report by Freud [34]. The few studies comparing the report lengths of different age groups yielded somewhat different results. Foulkes found a gradual increase in report length and narration throughout childhood concluding to reflect gradually-achieved developmental steps [29,39]. Colace also found difference in report lengths between three- to five- and five- to seven-year-olds’ narratives [48] in his home and school based studies but in the questionnaire based one he concluded that even preschoolers are able to form a short story of their dreams rather than just a sentence [66] similarly to five- to seven-year-olds. Behind these results one can observe the contradiction of laboratory versus home, school and questionnaire studies. When talking about narrative characteristics of dreams, we have to be aware that we can only be familiar with a secondary verbal expression of the original dream experience. In fact some authors claim that short and undetailed dream narratives could be a result of incomplete symbolization skills in young children [83] not necessarily reflecting the whole experience itself.

Preschoolers may only mention for example the ugliness of a dream character, and no other details that may be implied in the experience (for example experiencing threat, feeling fear, etc.), which would reflect the child’s verbal and symbolization skills rather than dreaming abilities. Others note that dream emotional tone could affect the likeliness of dream reports [27]. Given the inconsistent findings of children’s memories in connection with stressful events [118], it is hard to ascertain in what way emotional load in dreams would affect dream recalls, although it is reasonable to assume that it does therefore further research in this aspect is necessary. Finally as we observe the possible causes and confounders of dream report characteristics (implemented in the methodology and in the developmental level of children), we may conclude that deriving strong conclusions about children’s dreaming is premature at this point. A good example here could be Foulkes claiming validity only for laboratory studies and concluding perhaps prematurely that children under a certain age are not capable of dreaming.

Bizarreness of children’s dreams

Methods used for depicting bizarreness in children’s dreams, vary on a wide scale from simply detecting character and setting distortion [47] to applying complex multifaceted scales [100], thus drawing a direct comparison between the results could be highly problematic. In spite of these differences it is noteworthy that, contrary to Foulkes’ findings [39], which assumed no bizarreness in preschoolers’ dreams at all, others found a noticeable proportion of dreams with bizarre content (32% [48], 34% [30], 55.4% [66]). These results show that even young children are capable of the mental representation of bizarre or improbable events in their dreams (see Table 1), though this proportion is significantly lower than that of adults [30]. On the other hand these dreams of young children reinforce that bizarreness is not an inherent characteristic of REM dreams (therefore cannot exist solely as a consequence of random neurophysiologic phenomena of REM sleep as predicted by the activationeinput sourceemodulation (AIM) model [119]. In fact some findings suggest that cognitive development is also involved in achieving bizarreness in dreams. Colace [48] revealed associations between certain cognitive skills and dream bizarreness in children aged three to seven, and Resnick [30] demonstrated how different categories of bizarreness dominate in certain age groups suggesting parallelism with cognitive maturation.

Dream content and methodology

As we have seen above, the characteristics and content of children’s dreams observed by different studies seems to become more divergent with decreasing age. Approaching infancy there are fewer objective methods to investigate the nighttime experiences of the child. Consequently we still have no answer to the fundamental question: does dreaming exist in preverbal ages? Hypotheses about infant dreaming are formed indirectly: investigations on neural development [9,45] and observational case studies (for reviews see [85-87]) suggest dream experiences at these early ages; however inferences from cognitive psychological studies based on the dreaming of older children [29] predict the lack of dream experiences before the appropriate functioning of the visuo-spatial skills. The investigation of kindergarten-aged children produces a great variation in the data. Laboratory research reveals that the dreams of this age group are rare and mundane, with no emotions, no human characters or active self-representation, no kinematic imagery or narrative storyline [29]. In contrast, qualitative observational data show that young children’s dreams, in spite of being short and simple, depict a great variety of daytime experiences and have a strong connection with the child’s emotional struggles [34,36,90]. Quantitative outcomes of interview studies cohere with these latter findings suggesting that young children’s dreams are kinematic, filled with various human and non-human characters, depicting scenes of central and active self-participation [27,54]. Dream researchers agree that most of these differences are caused by the different methodologies and settings, but there has been a huge debate on which method yields the most representative results regarding young children’s dreams [29-31,42,46] (see also Supplementary Table S2). According to Foulkes the only reliable way of investigating dreams is in the neutral laboratory environment, which is free from parental suggestions, memory distortion and confabulation [29], although his results left some researchers with doubts [30,120]. Researchers with different methodological preferences blame the strange and unknown laboratory environment for posing a possible disorienting or restricting effect on the children (see section “Methods for collecting dream reports”) [30,46]. There is no doubt that the dream reports of children before adolescence are shorter and simpler than those collected from older children and adults. However, the static nature of young children’s dreams, as well as their lack of human characters, emotions and active self-representation was probably overestimated by the leaders of the laboratory studies [29]. If we consider the special features of narrative behavior in preschoolers (verbalizing only one relevant aspect of a story), together with the relative lack of differentiation between internal and external events [83] and the arbitrary nature of reporting anxiety dreams at all [27], the situation becomes even more modulated. As the gap between the results revealed by different methodologies seems to be much larger in young children than in any other age groups, the issue of the simplicity of children’s dreams has to be treated cautiously and analyzed critically. In conclusion, we do not wish to speak against the laboratory setting, instead we wish to emphasize that this method has its own drawbacks and advantages in a similar proportion as other settings. Every method will have a certain influence on the data and determine the results to a certain extent as dreaming is a physically and/or emotionally unique experience for each individual. Therefore we suggest that instead of searching for the most neutral or natural way of dream collection, or the typical “baseline dreams” of different age groups, one should consider any setting and method as a confounder on dreaming. Certainly different methods suit different research projects and aims more than others [120], we conclude that each of the described methods has its own relevance to developmental dream research. We believe that methodological pluralism leaves space for experiencing the spectrum and potentials of children’s dreams.

Nightmares and emotional processing

Importantly, nightmare studies converge on recognizing the role of emotional processing in dreaming throughout the lifespan. Early observational studies and later quantitative and qualitative research, using interviews, questionnaires or diaries, concluded that nightmares and bad dreams are closely connected to traumatic events [50,101,102,121], affective coping [51,122] or nighttime emotional reprocessing [90,123] in childhood as well as in adulthood, and this overarches different cultures [108,110]. Several studies demonstrated that the frequency of nightmare recall can be a correlate of current emotional difficulties (for example PTSD symptoms [101], anxiety levels [41,57,59] or exposure to trauma [121] or loss [124]) and that long-term emotional states and coping strategies are also reflected in certain dream characteristics [51,57,102]. The hypothesis that dreams have a role in reorganizing and reprocessing emotional experiences, is also a key concept in later neuro-cognitive theories supported by evidence from modern neuro-imaging techniques. Levin and Nielsen [123,125,126] emphasize the cooperation between subcortical areas conveying affective information and frontal-cognitive functions alleviating intense affective stimuli during dreaming. According to their model, the insufficient functioning of this affective-cognitive dialog may result in nightmares and in the long term it has a major role in developing and sustaining the recurrent nightmares in PTSD. An extensive review [112] concluded that the prevalence of nightmares in children is highest around the age of six to ten, decreasing thereafter. Some authors also found that nightmare occurrence is most common in childhood and adolescence through young adulthood and then declines with age [126]. Since frontal lobe functions are supposed to control and alleviate emotional loads during REM sleep and are among the last brain parts to fully develop [127], a plausible explanation lies in the immature nature of frontal executive functions. This hypothesis is supported by a recent finding showing impaired executive functioning characteristic for nightmare sufferers [128]. These coincident findings, showing a pattern of parallel progress in neuro-cognitive changes and dream patterns, draw attention to the need for further investigations regarding the correlations between these two related fields and to the importance of merging both adult and developmental dream literature.

Towards an extended neuro-cognitive dream theory

A number of empirical work on young children’s dreams support the hypothesis that developmental dreams deal with general emotional issues of the children rather than being simple wish fulfillments as Freud claimed [34] (see Appendix C). We suggest that interpreting dreams from the broader perspective of emotional reprocessing during the night is more understandable and plausible considering modern dream theories and could easily include the category of wish fulfillment (presented in Appendix C). For instance Freud’s example of little Hermann, who was asked to give the cherry basket to Freud, was probably having an emotional battle inside whether to obey to the adult order or do what comes naturally to a 22-mo-old: to eat the cherries. When he dreamt about eating all the cherries the next night he probably managed to re-process all his negative and unjust feelings towards the adults who forced him to give up on the cherries. If we consider all these everyday emotional turbulences and negative feelings of children, it is plausible to assume that they need some sort of re-processing or “extinction” in a way as Nielsen and Levin [123,126] suggest in their neuro-cognitive dream concept. Though the authors present their theory mainly by approaching the issue from nightmares and bad dreams as being indicators of problems in emotional regulation, their idea could be applied to dreaming in general. We should mention however, that the Freudian concepts on the function of dreams as guardians of sleep and as “safety valves” can be considered as forerunners of the fear extinction/emotional reprocessing theory of Nielsen and Levin [123,126]. Moreover, recent attempts to synthesize psychoanalysis with neurosciences are increasingly successful in reinterpreting Freudian concepts on children’s dreams. According to a new study emerging from this background child’s dreams often fulfill a wish originated in recent daytime, where it was associated to an intense affective state. Through the hallucinatory fulfillment, the dreams apparently resolve the associated affective state (the “affective-reestablishment” hypothesis) [129]. We suggest that developmental dream research gives an opportunity to support and to expand Nielsen and Levin’s theory on dreaming by further explaining the associations of neuro-cognitive changes and dream development. This inclusive hypothesis could lead us to the first step towards a unified neuro-cognitive dream theory on a conjoined basis of adult and developmental dream and nightmare research, incorporating dreaming over the lifespan.

Future directions for research

In order to clear up the confusingly diverse results from dream research of preschool children we need further investigation into the nature and characteristic of children’s dreams using carefully selected methods as well as a critical attitude when considering the inherent shortcomings of the research project. After obtaining a more coherent picture of the attributes of children’s dreams, the clarification of the cognitive, psychological, neural and sleep-EEG correlates of dream reports can be initialized. Preliminary and interesting results arise from the studies of Foulkes [29] on the correlation between dreaming and visuo-spatial development, or on the lack of correlation between memory performance and dream recall. Moreover, Colace [48] was able to demonstrate the correlation between general mental development and dream bizarreness in children. These preliminary results are encouraging to perform further research into mapping the role of cognitive functions and socio-emotional development in dreaming, and also emphasizing the importance of pluralism in dream assessment. Furthermore a deliberate characterization of the neurophysiological correlates of children’s dream behavior is also desperately needed. As we saw in the introduction, data in this field are scarce and contradictory, but results from nightmare studies and the hints of dream feature changes during preadolescent ages show that such investigations could be fruitful, increasing our knowledge in both neural and dream development. Based on hints from adult psycho-physiological studies, we believe that low neural connectivity and immature REM sleep eyemovement patterns are potential sources or correlates of the reported simplicity of children’s dream reports. Although we need more research on whether REM sleep eye-movement patterns or sleep EEG measures are correlated with the peculiarities in children’s dreams. There is also a lack of data on potential correlations between maturational measures of human sleep and the ontogeny of dreaming.

By revealing the core features of developmental and psychophysiological emergence of dreams our level of knowledge would increase in the fields of dream and developmental neuroscience and also contribute to the better understanding of the deep layers of human consciousness.

 

Research agenda

Since developmental patterns of dreaming are still divergent in the literature, more empirical research is needed for further clarification of this area.

1) Further characterization of the correlates of children’s dreaming and dream narratives, focusing not only on the cognitive abilities but also on the socio-emotional status and development (with special interest in the processing abilities of emotional stimuli).

2) Studying the neurophysiologic maturation as the basis of development in dreaming and dream narratives.

3) Revealing potential correlations between maturational measures of human sleep and the ontogeny of dreaming.

4) Further investigation of nightmares and possible background cognitive-emotional adjustment in childhood.

 

Practice points

When investigating children’s dreams we have to be careful about:

1) The methodological biases resulting in divergent outcomes, especially in case of young children (three- to five-year-olds).

2) Preschool children’s dream narratives might have the special feature of using one single narrative aspect to refer to a whole story (due to difficulties separating symbols from represented objects). This phenomenon might be most prominent in emotionally loaded dream experiences such as nightmares, resulting in a highly restricted account of nighttime experiences, thus making diagnosis difficult (for example nightmare versus pavor nocturnus).

3) Although results are divergent between and within age groups, the striking stability of findings on nightmares and their relationship with emotional disturbance/traumatic conditions proves the importance of emotional processing during sleep and dreaming. This should be recognized in pediatric sleep medicine and child psychiatry.

4) Parallelisms between neural changes and dream patterns in adolescent age direct our attention to the importance of general neural information processing and maturation in dreaming.

 

Conflict of interest

The authors have no conflict of interest to declare.

 

Acknowledgments

The work was supported by the 2010 Research Grant of the BIAL Foundation (55/10) and the Hungarian National Scientific Research Fund (OTKA-K105367).

 

Appendix A. A short summary on theories of dreaming with a particular focus on their developmental relevance

Here we aim to shed light on some of the main theoretical and conceptual frameworks of the psychophysiological emergence of dream experiences, with a special emphasis on their relevance on the ontogeny of dreaming. Our presentations are not focusing on the details and theoretical debates related to the particular theories. The reader can find relevant information on these aspects in the reference cited in the text.

1) Freudian dream theory. The Freudian topographical model is one of the early significant conceptual frameworks which try to explain the phenomenology of dreaming [34]. The model postulates that the normal waking flow of psychic energy from the perceptual to the motor subsystems is hindered during sleep (mainly because of inhibited motor output). The waking flow of psychic energy is presumed to be modulated by unconscious wishes and memories. Recent memories, called day residues are instigating these unconscious wishes during sleep, which e taken the fully inhibited motor output e results in the inverted flow of the psychic energy: from the unconscious wishes and memories to the perceptual side of the psychological system. This inverted flow is hypothesized to result in vivid imagery which manifests itself as dreaming. The structural side of the model (currently known as the disguisee censorship model) emphasizes the incompatibility between the psychological representation of unconscious wishes and the normative-moral rules governing the superego functions. This incompatibility is hypothesized to be worked out by the application of censorship on dream content and it is presumed to be accomplished by disguising the wishes and related representational contents. The disguised contents become unrecognizable and the resulting representations and plots bizarre. Thus, complex, bizarre dreams are mainly caused by the superego, which develops relatively late during ontogeny. Accordingly, children’s dreams are short and simple, while the underlying wishes can be easily recognized by the external observer. These predictions were supported by Freud’s early examples on children’s dreams as well as by some of the empirical research work reviewed in our paper. However, the overall level of simplicity and lack of bizarreness are still debated in the literature (see also the methodological issues detailed in the main text of our paper).

2) The activation-synthesis hypothesis later developed as the activationeinput sourceemodulation (AIM) model. The model is based on the neurochemical peculiarities of REM sleep. Neural activation during REM sleep is known to be characterized by burst-like, random brainstem cholinergic activity. In the 1970’s a group of Harvard scientists lead by Robert W. McCarley and Allan J. Hobson recognized that monoaminergic (noradrenergic and serotoninergic) modulation is strikingly absent during REM sleep [130]. The high levels of activation accompanied by monoaminergic demodulation were hypothesized to result in bizarre, psychosis-like hallucinatory activity resembling the effects of some psychedelic drugs. Moreover, activation initiated by the state-dependent increase in brainstem cholinergic activity is perceived and interpreted within the framework of previously stored memories and interpretation schemes, which is denoted as synthesis in the model [131]. The AIM model is a refined version of the theory, assuming that there are three dimensions determining the overall nature of behavioral states [119]. These are the following: neural activation (from low to high), input-source (which can vary from external to internal sources), and modulation (from cholinergic to aminergic dominance). The co-existence of high activation with internal input-source and low aminergic modulation is hypothesized to be characterized by bizarre and vivid visual imagery. The activation-synthesis hypothesis and the AIM model did not specifically instigated research on the ontogeny of dreaming. The physiological mechanism of REM sleep control is not known to change significantly during ontogeny. Although the synthesis part of the activationsynthesis hypothesis would be an ideal heuristic framework for studies on the ontogeny of dreaming, the fruitless debate between “physiological” and “cognitive” theories on dreaming hindered the realization of this possibility. On the other hand, leaders of the theory mainly emphasized the random physiological activation and aminergic demodulation as ageindependent conditions determining the bizarreness of dreams. Thus, supporters of the AIM model mostly emphasize the bizarreness of children’s dreams, arguing against the simplicity and realistic nature of the plots predicted by the psychoanalytic and cognitive-developmental theory (see later). Details of the levels of simplicity and bizarreness of children’s dreams are presented in the main text of our review.

3) The cognitive-developmental theory of dreaming. The only dream theory explicitly and primarily based on developmental data was formulated by David Foulkes [7,39]. The longitudinal and cross-sectional laboratory studies of Foulkes revealed characteristic developmental trends in different formal and content-related aspects of dreaming. As the content-related and the organizational aspects of dreams were shown to mirror the stages of cognitive development as described by Piaget [132], and the waking correlates of dreaming were mainly cognitive and visuo-spatial in nature, Foulkes concluded that dreaming reflects the constructive abilities of the children. In this view dreaming is merely a cognitive skill, the emergence of which can be described by the developmental stages of the theory of Jean Piaget [132]. Thus, dreaming is considered to be a gradually developing cognitive achievement. The internal physiological activation during sleep is considered as a neutral source constituting only the opportunity, but not the determining condition of dreaming. Several findings and consideration of this theory are presented and critically reviewed in the main text of our review. 4) The clinico-anatomical and neuro-psychoanalytic model of Solms. By an extensive review of the case reports and overall literature on the neurology of dreaming as well as by a clinical study on brain-damaged patients Mark Solms [133] revealed that REM sleep is neither necessary nor sufficient condition for dream generation. That is, the cholinergic brain stem mechanisms that control REM states can only generate the psychological phenomena of dreaming through the mediation of a second, probably dopaminergic, forebrain mechanism [134]. Moreover, specific forebrain structures including higher order association cortices, like the cortical areas surrounding the temporoparieto-occipital junction (Brodmann area 40), the medial temporo-occipital cortices, as well as the deep white matter structures of the frontal lobes were shown to be significant for the emergence of dreaming. The clinico-anatomical theory of dreaming supports the topographical model of Freud [34], as the evidence suggests the reverted direction of information flow during dreaming as compared to wakefulness: heteromodal associative cortices are activated first, while ulterior activation of secondary and primary sensory cortices are common. This is called as the backward projection mechanism by Solms [134]. As heteromodal higher order associative cortices are late maturing neuroanatomical structures the theory is ideally suited for formulating specific predictions on children’s dreams. This potential was not used, however. Hence the developmental aspects of dreaming remained largely unnoticed in the theory of Solms. Our review of the empirical findings might instigate this approach and could uncover some formerly unnoticed aspects of children’s dreams which could be confirmative or informative for the clinico-anatomical/ neuropsychoanalytic theory.

5) Fear extinction and the affective network dysfunction (AND) model. By incorporating findings in the areas of brain imaging, sleep physiology, post-traumatic stress disorder, anxiety disorders and the consolidation and extinction of fear memories Tore Nielsen and Ross Levin formulated a new neuro-cognitive model of nightmares with specific assumptions on dreaming in general [123]. The AND model assumes that the ineffectiveness of the medial prefrontal, as well as dorsal and rostral anterior cingulated cortices in dampening the strong affective reactions induced by the amygdale and the lack of fear extinction are leading to nightmare experiences. Fear extinction is hypothesized to be the key aspect of normal, non-dysphoric dreaming. Image contexts are provided by activation of the hippocampal formation. Based on the late maturation of medial prefrontal cortex and the resulting ineffectiveness in fear extinction the AND model predicts an increased nightmare frequency during childhood, which is indeed the case [126]. There is also some observational evidence for the inefficiency of dreaming-related fear extinction during early childhood in an observational case study cited in the main text of our review (see Observational studies under the heading Preverbal and early verbal dreams). The AND model is mainly predictive in terms of the affective aspects of the dream experiences.

 

Appendix B. Practical additions to the credibility of children’s dreams

Establishing an objective system to estimate the credibility of a child’s dream report is of primary importance for researchers dealing with this field. Since we cannot rule out the effect of walking fantasy on dream narratives especially reported upon spontaneous morning awakenings, reference points in order to distinguish between dream narratives and fantasy products are needed. Colace [48,84], in his attempt towards this goal established eight aspects of to consider as the sign of true dream report:

1) The child starts the report without hesitation.

2) The child reports the dream quickly in one go (although fulfilling these criteria could be difficult even for an adult when it comes to the recollection of possibly fragmented or bizarre dream content).

3) The child’s self definition of the story as a dream.

4) The placement of the experience itself during sleep period.

5) The coherence between the dream report and certain daytime experiences of the child in connection with the dream.

6) Good comprehension of the dream experience.

7) Consistency between specific dream report and the general concept of dream.

8) The last point introduces a new channel of story telling through drawing, which should be consistent with the verbal channel even after a certain time lapse. This latter method was shown to be successful indicator of memorable and personally important dream reports [135].

Obtaining this information would of course require a thorough interview with the child. Although dream report styles could possibly show great variation across subjects, consistency of the narrative with other narratives, concepts and means of expression of the same child could be a useful and valid indicator of dream recall. As Colace points out “it is useful to collect many of these indicatory of dream report credibility and to observe their convergence” [p. 80].

Appendix C. A note on the function of dreams

Theoretical works on the functions of dreams based on developmental dream research is extremely scarce. One of the sporadic hypotheses of the possible functions of children’s dreams is derived from Freud [34]. Based on various spontaneous dream reports from children Freud [34] concluded: “The dreams of little children are often simple fulfillments of wishes.” [p. 108]. This idea became popular as many authors found evidence in children’s dream reports [27,48,63,136]. However other researchers approached the content of these dreams somewhat differently [96]: “.the present results do not seem to substantiate the theory that dreams are primarily a form of wish fulfillment.” [p. 133] concluded Jersild and colleagues after examining 400 dream reports of children aged between five- to twelve-years. Also some observational studies concluded that the spectrum of themes of children’s dreams is broader than being simple wish fulfillments [32,36,90]. They tried to extend this concept by stating that all emotionally relevant wakeful experiences are prone to appear in dreams not primarily because of their wishful aspect but because children are often “struggling with strong and strange emotions which he could not work through during the excitement and rapidity of reality” [p. 512] so they continue the work in their dreams [35]. Colace [48], who addressed the wish fulfillment aspect of children’s dreams, states that “the origin of the wishes [.] is represented by an intense emotional daytime experience of the child” [p. 163] and that wish fulfillment in dreams serves “an affective resolution function” [p. 180] based on Freud’s theories and his own research. We suggest that the above definition of wish fulfillment could be interpreted under the larger concept of emotional reprocessing and coping, as we paraphrase in the main text.

 

Appendix D. Supplementary data

Supplementary data related to this article can be found online at http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.smrv.2014.02.001.

 

References

[1] Dan B, Boyd SG. A neurophysiological perspective on sleep and its maturation. Dev Med Child Neurol 2007;48:773-9.

[2] Eisermann M, Kaminska A, Moutard M-L, Soufflet C, Plouin P. Normal EEG in childhood: from neonates to adolescents. Clin Neurophysiol 2013;43: 35-65.

[3] Danker-Hopfe H. Growth and development of children with a special focus on sleep. Prog Biophys Mol Biol 2011;107:333e8.

[4] Hobson JA. Consciousness, sleep, and dreaming. In: Encyclopedia of cognitive science; 2006.

[5] Hobson JA, Pace-Schott EF, Stickgold R. Dreaming and the brain: toward a cognitive neuroscience of conscious states. Behav Brain Sci 2000;23:793e 842.

[6] Kramer M. Dreaming has content and meaning not just form. Behav Brain Sci 2000;23:959e61.

[7] Foulkes D. Dreaming: a cognitive-psychological analysis. Hillsdale, New Jersey: L. Erlbaum Associates; 1985.

[8] Aserinsky E, Kleitman N. Regularly occurring periods of eye motility, and concomitant phenomena, during sleep. Science 1953;118:273e4.

[9] Staunton H. The function of dreaming. Rev Neurosci 2001;12:365e71.

[10] Jenni OG, Dahl RE. Sleep, cognition, and emotion: a developmental view. In: Nelson CA, Luciana M, editors. Handbook of developmental cognitive neuroscience. 2nd ed. Cambridge: MIT Press; 2008. pp. 807e17.

[11] Grigg-Damberger M, Gozal D, Marcus CL, Quan SF, Rosen CL, Chervin RD, et al. The visual scoring of sleep and arousal in infants and children. J Clin Sleep Med 2007;3:201e40.

[12] Blumberg MS, Lucas DE. A developmental and component analysis of active sleep. Dev Psychobiol 1996;29:1e22.

[13] Hong CC, Potkin SG, Antrobus JS, Dow BM, Callaghan GM, Gillin JC. REM sleep eye movement counts correlate with visual imagery in dreaming: a pilot study. Psychophysiology 1997;34:377e81.

[14] Berger RJ, Oswald I. Eye movements during active and passive dreams. Science 1962;137:601.

[15] Foulkes D, Bradley L. Phasic activity and dream recall in 5- to 8-yr.-olds. Percept Mot Skills 1989;69:290.

[16] Aserinsky E. Relationship of rapid eye movement density to the prior accumulation of sleep and wakefulness. Psychophysiology 1973;10: 545e58.

[17] Lucidi F, Devoto A, Violani C, De Gennaro L, Mastracci P, Bertini M. Rapid eye movements density as a measure of sleep need: REM density decreases linearly with the reduction of prior sleep duration. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol 1996;99:556e61.

[18] De Gennaro L, Marzano C, Moroni F, Curcio G, Ferrara M, Cipolli C. Recovery sleep after sleep deprivation almost completely abolishes dream recall. Behav Brain Res 2010;206:293e8.

[19] Quadens O. Order and disorder in the brain function. Neuroendocrinol Lett 2003;24:151e60.

[20] Als H, Duffy FH, McAnulty GB, Rivkin MJ, Vajapeyam S, Mulkern RV, et al. Early experience alters brain function and structure. Pediatrics 2004;113: 846e57.

[21] Barry RJ, Clarke AR, McCarthy R, Selikowitz M, Johnstone SJ, Rushby JA. Age and gender effects in EEG coherence: I. Developmental trends in normal children. Clin Neurophysiol 2004;115:2252e8.

[22] Grossmann T, Johnson MH. The development of the social brain in human infancy. Eur J Neurosci 2007;25:909e19.

[23] Nielsen TA, Chénier V. Variations in EEG coherence as an index of the affective content of dreams from REM sleep: relationships with face imagery. Brain Cogn 1999;41:200e12.

[24] Chellappa SL, Frey S, Knoblauch V, Cajochen C. Cortical activation patterns herald successful dream recall after NREM and REM sleep. Biol Psychol 2011;87:251e6.

[25] Feinberg I, Campbell IG. Longitudinal sleep EEG trajectories indicate complex patterns of adolescent brain maturation. Am J Physiol Integr Comp Physiol 2013;304:R296e303.

[26] Kramer M. The assessment of dream content: methodological considerations. Sleep Med Clin 2010;5:183-91.

*[27] Despert LJ. Dreams in children of preschool age. Psychoanal Study Child 1949;3:141-80.

[28] DeMartino MF. A review of the literature on children’s dreams. Psychiatr Q Suppl 1955;29:90-101.

*[29] Foulkes D. Children’s dreaming and the development of consciousness. Cambridge, Massachusetts: Harvard University Press; 1999.

*[30] Resnick J, Stickgold R, Rittenhouse CD, Hobson JA. Self-representation and bizarreness in children’s dream reports collected in the home setting. Conscious Cogn 1994;3:30-45.

[31] Domhoff B, Kamiya J. Problems in dream content study with objective indicators I. A comparison of home and laboratory dream reports. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1964;11:519-24.

[32] Erickson MH. On the possible occurrence of a dream in an eight-monthold infant. Psychoanal Q 1941;10:382-4.

[33] Fraiberg S. On the sleep disturbances of early childhood. Psychoanal Study Child 1950;5:285-309.

[34] Freud S. The interpretation of dreams (1900). 3rd ed. New York: The Macmillan Company; 1913.

[35] Grotjahn M. Dream observations in a two-year-four-months-old baby Psychoanal Q 1938;7:507-13.

[36] Niederland WG. The earliest dreams of a young child. Psychoanal Study Child 1957;12:190-208.

[37] Piaget J. Play, dreams and imitation in childhood [La formation du symbole chez l’enfant, 1945]. Routledge; 1999.

[38] Nielsen T, Svob C, Kuiken D. Dream-enacting behaviors in a normal population. Sleep 2009;32:1629-36.

[39] Foulkes D. Children’s dreams: longitudinal studies. John Wiley & Sons Inc.; 1982.

[40] Foulkes D, Hollifield M, Sullivan B, Bradley L, Terry R. REM dreaming and cognitive skills at ages 5e8: a cross-sectional study. Int J Behav Dev 1990;13:447-65.

[41] Foulkes D, Larson JD, Swanson EM, Rardin M. Two studies of childhood dreaming. Am J Orthopsychiatr 1969;39:627-43.

[42] Foulkes D. Home and laboratory dreams: four empirical studies and a conceptual reevaluation. Sleep 1979;2:233-51.

[43] Foulkes D. Dreams of the male child: four case studies. J Child Psychol Psychiatry Allied Discipl 1967;8:81-98.

[44] Foulkes D, Pivik T, Steadman HS, Spear PS, Symonds JD. Dreams of the male child: an EEG study. J Abnorm Psychol 1967;72:457-67.

*[45] Burnham MM, Conte C. Developmental perspective dreaming across the lifespan and what this tells us. Int Rev Neurobiol 2010;92:47-68.

[46] Domhoff B. Home dreams versus laboratory dreams: home dreams are better. In: Kramer M, editor. Dream psychology and the new biology of dreaming. Springfield: Charles C Thomas; 1969. pp. 199-217.

[47] Foulkes D, Shepherd J. Manual for a scoring system for children’s dreams; 1971. p. 151.

*[48] Colace C. Children’s dreams: from Freud’s observations to modern dream research. 1st ed. London: Karnac Books Ltd.; 2010.

[49] Strauch I, Lederbogen S. The home dreams and waking fantasies of boys and girls between ages 9 and 15: a longitudinal study. Dreaming 1999;9: 153-61.

[50] Punamäki R-L, Ali KJ, Ismahil KH, Nuutinen J. Trauma, dreaming, and psychological distress among Kurdish children. Dreaming 2005;15: 178-94.

[51] Helminen E, Punamäki R-L. Contextualized emotional images in children’s dreams: psychological adjustment in conditions of military trauma. Int J Behav Dev 2008;32:177-87.

[52] Muris P, Herckelbach H, Gadet B, Moulaert V, Merckelbach H. Fears, worries, and scary dreams in 4- to 12-year-old children: their content, developmental pattern, and origins. J Clin Child Psychol 2000;29:43-52.

[53] Beaudet D. Encountering the monster: pathways in children’s dreams. 1st ed. Continuum Intl Pub Group; 1990.

*[54] Honig AS, Nealis AL. What do young children dream about? Early Child Dev Care 2012;182:771-95.

[55] Adams K. God talks to me in my dreams: the occurrence and significance of children’s dreams about god. Int J Child Spiritual 2001;6:99-111.

*[56] Schredl M, Blomeyer D, Görlinger M. Nightmares in children: influencing factors. Somnol Schlafforsch Schlafmed 2000;4:145-9.

[57] Schredl M, Pallmer R, Montasser A. Anxiety dreams in school-aged children. Dreaming 1996;6:265-70.

[58] Nielsen T, Laberge L, Paquet J, Tremblay RE, Vitaro F, Montplaisir J. Development of disturbing dreams during adolescence and their relation to anxiety symptoms. Sleep 2000;23:727-36.

 

*[59] Simard V, Nielsen T, Tremblay RE, Boivin M, Montplaisir JIY. Longitudinal study of bad dreams in preschool-aged children: prevalence, demographic correlates, risk and protective factors. Sleep 2008;31:62-70.

[60] Li SX, Yu MWM, Lam SP, Zhang J, Li AM, Lai KYC, et al. Frequent nightmares in children: familial aggregation and associations with parentreported behavioral and mood problems. Sleep 2011;34:487-93.

[61] Avila-White D, Schneider A, Domhoff GW. The most recent dreams of 12e 13 year-old boys and girls: a methodological contribution to the study of dream content in teenagers. Dreaming 1999;9:163-71.

[62] Crugnola CR, Maggiolini A, Caprin C, De Martini C, Giudici F. Dream content of 10- to 11-year-old preadolescent boys and girls. Dreaming 2008;18:201-16.

[63] Kimmins CW. Children’s dreams. London: Longmans, Green and Co.; 1920.

[64] Oberst U, Charles C, Chamarro A. Influence of gender and age in aggressive dream content of Spanish children and adolescents. Dreaming 2005;15: 170-7.

[65] Saline S. The most recent dreams of children ages 8-11. Dreaming 1999;9: 173-81.

[66] Colace C. Children’s dreaming: a study based on questionnaires completed by parents. Sleep Hypn 2006;8:19-32.

[67] Schredl M, Fricke-Oerkermann L, Mitschke A, Wiater A, Lehmkuhl G. Factors affecting nightmares in children: parents’ vs. children’s ratings. Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry 2009;18:20-5.

[68] Domhoff GW. The scientific study of dreams: neural networks, cognitive development, and content analysis. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association; 2003.

[69] Domhoff GW. Finding meaning. In: Dreams: a quantitative approach. New York: Plenum Press; 1996.

[70] Schredl M, Fricke-Oerkermann L, Mitschke A, Wiater A, Lehmkuhl G. Longitudinal study of nightmares in children: stability and effect of emotional symptoms. Child Psychiatry Hum Dev 2009;40:439-49.

[71] Soffer-Dudek N, Sadeh A. Dream recall frequency and unusual dream experiences in early adolescence: longitudinal links to behavior problems. J Res Adolesc 2013;23:635-61.

[72] Domhoff B, Kamiya J. Problems in dream content study with objective indicators II. Appearance of experimental situation in laboratory dream narratives. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1964;11:525-8.

[73] Weisz R, Foulkes D. Home and laboratory dreams collected under uniform sampling conditions. Psychophysiology 1970;6:588-96.

[74] Hall CS, Van de Castle RL. The content analysis of dreams. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts; 1966.

[75] Piaget J. The child’s conception of the world. London: Rowman & Littlefield; 1929.

[76] Woolley JD, Wellman HM. Children’s conceptions of dreams. Cogn Dev 1992;7:365-80.

[77] Meyer S, Shore C. Children’s understanding of dreams as mental states. Dreaming 2001;11:179-94.

[78] Woolley JD, Boerger EA. Development of beliefs about the origins and controllability of dreams. Dev Psychol 2002;38:24-41.

[79] Kinoshita T. Young children’s understanding of mental representation: pretend and dream. Psychol e Int J Psychol Orient 1994;37:1-6.

[80] Sharon T, Woolley JD. Do monsters dream? Young children’s understanding of the fantasy/reality distinction. Br J Dev Psychol 2004;22: 293-310.

[81] Cassi V, Pinto G, Salzarulo P. Developmental changes of children’s about sleep and dreaming. Sleep Res Online 1999;2(Suppl. 1):194.

[82] Colace C. Dream bizarreness reconsidered. Sleep Hypn 2003;5:105-28.

[83] Bauer DH. An exploratory study of developmental changes in children’s fears. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 1976;17:69-75.

[84] Colace C. Sulla valutazione della credibilità dei sogni raccontati dai bambini: uno studio preliminare. Psychiatr Infanz Adolesc 1998;65:5-18.

[85] Ablon SL, Mack JE. Children’s dreams reconsidered. Psychoanal Study Child 1980;35:179-217.

[86] Murray JB. Children’s dreams. J Genet Psychol 1995;156:303-12.

[87] Wilkerson DC. Children’s dreams 1900-1980. Annu Psychoanal 1981;9: 57-71.

[88] von Hug-Hellmuth H. A study of the mental life of the child. Washington: Nervous and Mental Disease Publishing Company; 1919.

[89] Kohler WC, Coddington RD, Agnew HW. Sleep patterns in 2-year-old children. J Pediatr 1968;72:228-33.

[90] Anderson JE. The dream as a re-conditioning process. J Abnorm Soc Psychol 1927;22:21-5.

[91] Foster JC, Anderson JE. Unpleasant dreams in childhood. Child Dev 1936;7: 77-84.

[92] Foulkes D. Dreaming and cognitive development. In: Montangero Jacques, Tryphon A, Dionnet S, editors. Symbolisme et connaissance/Symbolism and knowledge. Genève: Fondation Archives Jean Piaget; 1987. pp. 177-95.

[93] Hawkins C, Williams TI. Nightmares, life events and behaviour problems in preschool children. Child Care Health Dev 1992;18:117-28.

[94] Colace C. Self-representation in young children’s dream reports. Sleep 2000;23(Suppl. 2):A176-7.

[95] Colace C, Violani C, Tuci B. Self representation in dreams reported from young children at school. In: Sleep research. Los Angeles: University of California; 1995. p. 69.

[96] Jersild AT, Markey FV, Jersild CL. Children’s fears, dreams, wishes, daydreams, likes, dislikes, pleasant and unpleasant memories: a study by the interview method of 400 children aged 5 to 12. Child Deve. Teachers College, Columbia University; 1933.

[97] Colace C, Ferendeles R, Tuci B, Celani G. Children’s dreaming: a study based on questionnaire compiled by parents. Sleep 2000;23(Suppl. 2):A175.

[98] Levi G, Pompilli E. La narrazione del racconto del sogno in bambini in eta prescolare [Dream story telling in preschool age children]. Psichiatr Infanz Adolesc 1991;58:517-25.

[99] Colace C, Violani C, Solano L. La deformazione-bizzarria onirica nella teoria freudiana del sogno: indicazioni teoriche e verifica di due ipotesi di ricerca in un campione di 50 sogni di bambini. Arch Psicol Neurol Psichiatr 1993;54:380-401.

[100] Rittenhouse, Cynthia D, Stickgold R, Hobson JA. Constraint on the transformation of characters, objects, and settings in dream reports. Conscious Cogn 1994;3:100-13.

[101] Wittmann L, Zehnder D, Schredl M, Jenni OG, Landolt MA. Posttraumatic nightmares and psychopathology in children after road traffic accidents. J Trauma Stress 2010;23:232-9.

[102] Terr LC. Children of Chowchilla: a study of psychic trauma. Psychoanal Study Child 1979;34:547-623.

[103] Strauch I. REM dreaming in the transition from late childhood to adolescence: a longitudinal study. Dreaming 2005;15:155-69.

[104] Trupin EW. Correlates of ego-level and agency-communion in stage REM dreams of 11e13 year old children. J Child Psychol Psychiatry Allied Discipl 1976;17:169-80.

[105] McNamara P, McLaren D, Durso K. Representation of the self in REM and NREM dreams. Dreaming 2007;17:113-26.

[106] Bruni O, Lo Reto F, Recine A, Ottaviano S, Guidetti V. Development and validation of a dream content questionnaire for school age children. Sleep Hypn 1999;1:41-6.

[107] Punamäki R-L. The relationship of dream content and changes in daytime mood in traumatized vs. non-traumatized children. Dreaming 1999;9:213-33.

[108] Bilu Y. The other as a nightmare: the Israeli-Arab in children’s encounter as reflected dreams in Israel and the West Bank. Polit Psychol 2013;10: 365-89.

[109] Valli K, Revonsuo A, Pälkäs O, Ismail KH, Ali KJ, Punamäki R-L. The threat simulation theory of the evolutionary function of dreaming: evidence from dreams of traumatized children. Conscious Cogn 2005;14:188-218.

[110] Levine JB. The role of culture in the representation of conflict in dreams: a comparison of Bedouin, Irish, and Israeli Children. J Cross Cult Psychol 1991;22:472-90.

[111] Schredl M, Pallmer R. Geschlechtsspezifische Unterschiede in Angstträumen von Schülerinen und Schülern [Sex specific differences of nightmares in students]. Prax Kinderpsychol Kinderpsychiatr 1998;47:463-76.

[112] Schredl M, Pallmer R. Alpträume bei Kindern [Nightmares in children]. Prax Kinderpsychol Kinderpsychiatr 1997;46:36-56.

[113] Mindell JA, Barrett KM. Nightmares and anxiety in elementary-aged children: is there a relationship? Child Care Health Dev 2002;28:317-22.

[114] Schredl M, Sartorius H. Dream recall and dream content in children with attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Child Psychiatry Hum Dev 2010;41:230-8.

[115] Viemerö V, Paajanen S. The role of fantasies and dreams in the TV viewingaggression relationship. Aggress Behav 1992;18:109-16.

[116] Schredl M, Anders A, Hellriegel S, Rehm A. TV viewing, computer game playing and nightmares in school children. Dreaming 2008;18:69-76.

[117] Schredl M, Buscher A, Haaß C, Scheuermann M, Uhrig K. Gender differences in dream socialization in children and adolescence. Int J Adolesc Youth; 2013:1-8 [in press].

[118] Quas JA, Goodman GS, Bidrose S, Pipe ME, Craw S, Ablin DS. Emotion and memory: children’s long-term remembering, forgetting, and suggestibility. J Exp Child Psychol 1999;72:235-70.

[119] Hobson JA, Friston KJ. Waking and dreaming consciousness: neurobiological and functional considerations. Prog Neurobiol 2012;98:82-98.

[120] Bulkeley K, Broughton B, Sanchez A, Stiller J. Earliest remembered dreams. Dreaming 2005;15:205-22.

[121] Nader K. Children’s traumatic dreams. In: Barrett D, editor. Trauma and dreams. London: Harvard University Press; 1996. p. 272.

[122] Lavie P, Kaminer H. Sleep, dreaming and coping style in holocaust survivors. In: Barrett D, editor. Trauma and dreams. London: Harvard University Press; 1996. p. 272.

[123] Nielsen T, Levin R. Nightmares: a new neurocognitive model. Sleep Med Rev 2007;11:295-310.

[124] Cooper CA. Children’s dreams during the grief process. Prof Sch Couns 1999;3:137-40.

[125] Levin R, Nielsen T. Nightmares, bad dreams and emotion dysregulation. Curr Dir Psychol Sci 2009;18:84-8.

*[126] Levin R, Nielsen TA. Disturbed dreaming, posttraumatic stress disorder, and affect distress: a review and neurocognitive model. Psychol Bull 2007;133:482-528.

[127] Thompson-Schill SL, Ramscar M, Chrysikou EG. Cognition without control: when a little frontal lobe goes a long way. Curr Dir Psychol Sci 2009;18: 259-63.

[128] Simor P, Pajkossy P, Horváth K, Bódizs R. Impaired executive functions in subjects with frequent nightmares as reflected by performance in different neuropsychological tasks. Brain Cogn 2012;78:274-83.

[129] Colace C. Are the wish-fulfillment dreams of children the royal road for looking at the functions of dreams? Neuropsychoanalysis 2013;15:161-75.

[130] Hobson J, McCarley R, Wyzinski P. Sleep cycle oscillation: reciprocal discharge by two brainstem neuronal groups. Science 1975;189:55-8.

[131] Hobson JA. The brain as a dream state generator: an activation-synthesis hypothesis of the dream process. Am J Psychiatry 1977;134:1335-48.

[132] Piaget J. Piaget’s theory. In: Inhälder B, Chipman H, editors. Piaget and his school. Berlin: Springer; 1976. pp. 11-23.

[133] Solms M. The neuropsychology of dreams: a clinico-anatomical study. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers; 1997.

[134] Solms M. Dreaming and REM sleep are controlled by different brain mechanisms. Behav Brain Sci 2000;23:843-50.

[135] Lurçat L. Une approche de l’espace mental: le dessin du rêve chez l’enfant [An approach to mental space: drawing of dreams in children]. J Psychol Norm Pathol 1977;74:293e310.

[136] Hill JC. Education and dreams. London: Methuen & Co.; 1926.

 

Supplementary Table S1 Table S1 Examples of dream reports of different age groups assessed using various methodologies.

 suppl-table1-1suppl-table1-2

  1. Piaget J. Play, Dreams and Imitation in Childhood [La formation du symbole chez l’enfant, 1945]. Routledge; 1999.
  2. Freud S. The Interpretation of Dreams (1900). third edit. New York: The Macmillan Company; 1913.
  3. Niederland WG. The Earliest Dreams of a Young Child. Psychoanal Study Child 1957;12:190–208.
  4. Colace C. Children ’ s Dreaming : A Study Based on Questionnaires Completed by Parents. Sleep Hypn 2006;8:19–32.
  5. Colace C. Children’s Dreams: From Freud’s Observations to Modern Dream Research. 1st ed. London: Karnac Books Ltd.; 2010.
  6. Honig AS, Nealis AL. What do young children dream about? Early Child Dev Care 2012;182:771–95.
  7. Foulkes D. Children’s Dreaming and the Development of Consciousness. Cambridge, Massachusetts: Harvard University Press; 1999.
  8. Saline S. The Most Recent Dreams of Children Ages 8-11. Dreaming 1999;9:173–81.
  9. Levine JB. The Role of Culture in the Representation of Conflict in Dreams: A Comparison of Bedouin, Irish, and Israeli Children. J Cross Cult Psychol 1991;22:472–90.
  10. Strauch I. REM dreaming in the transition from late childhood to adolescence: A longitudinal study. Dreaming 2005;15:155–69.

 

Supplementary Table S2

Table S2 Summary of dream collection methods used in developmental dream research.

 suppl-table-2

Supplementary Table S3

Table S3. A collection of dream research in children assorted by typical dream collection methods and settings given the studied age groups.

 

 suppl-table-4

  1. Foulkes D, Hollifield M, Sullivan B, Bradley L, Terry R. REM Dreaming and Cognitive Skills at Ages 5-8: A Cross-sectional Study. Int J Behav Dev 1990;13:447–65.
  2. Foulkes D, Pivik T, Steadman HS, Spear PS, Symonds JD. Dreams of the Male Child: An EEG Study. J Abnorm Psychol 1967;72:457–67.
  3. Foulkes D, Larson JD, Swanson EM, Rardin M. Two Studies of Childhood Dreaming. Am J Orthopsychiat 1969 39:627–43.
  4. Foulkes D. Children’s Dreams: Longitudinal Studies. John Wiley & Sons Inc; 1982.
  5. Foulkes D. Children’s Dreaming and the Development of Consciousness. Cambridge, Massachusetts: Harvard University Press; 1999.
  6. Pierce CM, Whitman R, Maas J, Gay M. Enuresis And Dreaming: Experimental studies. Arch Gen Psychiat 1961;4:166–70.
  7. Strauch I. REM dreaming in the transition from late childhood to adolescence: A longitudinal study. Dreaming 2005;15:155–69.
  8. Resnick J, Stickgold R, Rittenhouse CD, Hobson JA. Self-representation and bizarreness in children’s dream reports collected in the home setting. Conscious Cogn 1994;3:30–45.
  9. Colace C. Children’s Dreams: From Freud’s Observations to Modern Dream Research. 1st ed. London: Karnac Books Ltd.; 2010.
  10. Strauch I, Lederbogen S. The home dreams and waking fantasies of boys and girls between ages 9 and 15: A longitudinal study. Dreaming 1999;9:153–61.
  11. Helminen E, Punamäki R-L. Contextualized emotional images in children’s dreams: Psychological adjustment in conditions of military trauma. Int J Behav Dev 2008;32:177–87.
  12. Punamäki R-L. The Relationship of Dream Content and Changes in Daytime Mood in Traumatized vs. Non-Traumatized Children. Dreaming 1999;9:213–33.
  13. Punamäki R-L, Ali KJ, Ismahil KH, Nuutinen J. Trauma, dreaming, and psychological distress among Kurdish children. Dreaming 2005;15:178–94.
  14. Proksch K, Schredl M. Impact of Parental Divorce on Children’s Dreams. J Divorce Remarriage 1999;30:71–82.
  15. Bilu Y. The Other as a Nightmare: The Israeli-Arab in Children’s Encounter as Reflected Dreams in Israel and the West Bank. Polit Psychol 2013;10:365–89.
  16. Colace C. Children ’ s Dreaming : A Study Based on Questionnaires Completed by Parents. Sleep Hypn 2006;8:19–32.
  17. Simard V, Nielsen T, Tremblay RE, Boivin M, Montplaisir JIY. Longitudinal Study of Bad Dreams in Preschool-Aged Children: Prevalence,Demographic Correlates, Risk and Protective Factors. Sleep 2008;31:62–70.
  18. Foster JC, Anderson JE. Unpleasant Dreams in Childhood. Child Dev 1936;7:77–84.
  19. Li SX, Yu MWM, Lam SP, Zhang J, Li AM, Lai KYC, et al. Frequent nightmares in children: familial aggregation and associations with parent-reported behavioral and mood problems. Sleep 2011;34:487–93.
  20. Hawkins C, Williams TI. Nightmares, life events and behaviour problems in preschool children. Child Care Health Dev 1992;18:117–28.
  21. Schredl M, Blomeyer D, Görlinger M. Nightmares in Children : Influencing Factors. Somnologie 2000;4:145–9.
  22. Mindell JA;., Barrett KM. Nightmares and anxiety in elementary-aged children: is there a relationship? Child Care Health Dev 2002;28:317–22.
  23. Schredl M, Fricke-Oerkermann L, Mitschke A, Wiater A, Lehmkuhl G. Longitudinal study of nightmares in children: stability and effect of emotional symptoms. Child Psychiatry Hum Dev 2009;40:439–49.
  24. Schredl M, Fricke-Oerkermann L, Mitschke A, Wiater A, Lehmkuhl G. Factors affecting nightmares in children: parents’ vs. children’s ratings. Eur Child Adolescent Psychiatry 2009;18:20–5.
  25. Soffer-Dudek N, Sadeh A. Dream Recall Frequency and Unusual Dream Experiences in Early Adolescence: Longitudinal Links to Behavior Problems. J Res Adolescence 2013;23:635–61.
  26. Despert LJ. Dreams in Children of Preschool Age. Psychoanal Stud Chil 1949;3:141–80.
  27. Honig AS, Nealis AL. What do young children dream about? Early Child Dev Care 2012;182:771– 95.
  28. Kimmins CW. Children’s dreams. London: Longmans,Green and Co.; 1920.
  29. Adams K. God Talks to Me in My Dreams: The occurrence and significance of children’s dreams about God. Int J Child Spiritual 2001;6:99–111.
  30. Muris P, Herckelbach H, Gadet B, Moulaert V, Merckelbach H. Fears, Worries, and Scary Dreams in 4- to 12-Year-Old Children:Their Content, Developmental Pattern, and Origins. J Clin Child Psychol 2000;29:43–52.
  31. Wittmann L, Zehnder D, Schredl M, Jenni OG, Landolt MA. Posttraumatic Nightmares and Psychopathology in Children After Road Traffic Accidents. J Trauma Stress 2010;23:232–9.
  32. Bauer DH. An Exploratory Study of Developmental Changes in Children’s Fears. J Child Psychol Psyc 1976;17:69–75.
  33. Terr LC. Children of Chowchilla: a study of psychic trauma. Psychoanal Stud Chil 1979;34:547– 623.
  34. Levine JB. The Role of Culture in the Representation of Conflict in Dreams: A Comparison of Bedouin, Irish, and Israeli Children. J Cross Cult Psychol 1991;22:472–90.
  35. Schredl M, Pallmer R, Montasser A. Anxiety Dreams in School-Aged Children. Dreaming 1996;6:265–70.
  36. Epstein AL. Strange Encounters: Dreams and Nightmares of High School Students in Papua New Guinea. Oceania 1998;68:200–12.
  37. Nielsen T, Laberge L, Paquet J, Tremblay RE, Vitaro F, Montplaisir J. Development of Disturbing Dreams During Adolescence and Their Relation to Anxiety Symptoms. Sleep 2000;23:727–36.
  38. Brand S, Beck J, Kalak N, Gerber M, Kirov R, Pühse U, et al. Dream recall and its relationship to sleep, perceived stress, and creativity among adolescents. J Adolesc Health 2011;49:525–31.
  39. Schredl M, Anders A, Hellriegel S, Rehm A. TV viewing, computer game playing and nightmares in school children. Dreaming 2008;18:69–76.
  40. Schredl M, Buscher A, Haaß C, Scheuermann M, Uhrig K, Haas C. Gender Differences in Dream Socialization in Children and Adolescence. Int J Adolesc Youth 2013;“in press”:1–8.
  41. Avila-White D, Schneider A, Domhoff GW. The Most Recent Dreams of 12-13 Year-Old Boys and Girls : A Methodological Contribution to the Study of Dream Content in Teenagers. Dreaming 1999;9:163–71.
  42. Crugnola CR, Maggiolini A, Caprin C, Martini C De, Giudici F. Dream content of 10- to 11-year-old preadolescent boys and girls. Dreaming 2008;18:201–16.
  43. Oberst U, Charles C, Chamarro A. Influence of gender and age in aggressive dream content of Spanish children and adolescents. Dreaming 2005;15:170–7.
  44. Saline S. The Most Recent Dreams of Children Ages 8-11. Dreaming 1999;9:173–81.
  45. Schredl M, Sartorius H. Dream recall and dream content in children with attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Child Psychiat Hum D 2010;41:230–8. 46. Fiske KE, Pillemer DB. Adult recollections of earliest childhood dreams: a cross-cultural study. Memory 2006;14:57–67.
  46. Bulkeley K, Broughton B, Sanchez A, Stiller J. Earliest remembered dreams. Dreaming 2005;15:205–22.

 

Supplementary Table S4. Collection and summary of some important observational and case studies from the last century Author

 

  1. Freud S. The Interpretation of Dreams. S.E., 4-5. London: Hogarth Press; 1900.
  2. Freud S. The “Wolfman” and Other Cases. Penguin Books; 2002.
  3. Hug-Hellmuth H von. A Study of the Mental Life of the Child. Washington: Nervous And Mental Disease Publishing Company; 1919.
  4. Anderson JE. The dream as a re-conditioning process. J Abnorm Soc Psychol 1927;22:21–5.
  5. Grotjahn M. Dream Observations in a Two-Year-Four-Months-Old Baby. Psychoanal Quart 1938;7:507–13.
  6. Erickson MH. On the Possible Occurrence of a Dream in an Eight-Month-Old Infant. Psychoanal Quart 1941;10:382–4.
  7. Fraiberg S. On the Sleep Disturbances of Early Childhood. Psychoanal Stud Chil 1950;5:285–309.
  8. Niederland WG. The Earliest Dreams of a Young Child. Psychoanal Study Child 1957;12:190–208.